Stimulus similarity decreases spontaneous object recognition regardless of the retention interval in rats
- Rocío Angulo 1
- Naiara Arriola 2
- Gumersinda Alonso 2
-
1
Universidad Autónoma de Chile
info
- 2 Universidad del País Vasco, UPV/EHU
ISSN: 1576-8597
Year of publication: 2017
Volume: 38
Issue: 2
Pages: 195-208
Type: Article
More publications in: Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental
Abstract
The experiment aimed to test in a spontaneous object recognition (SOR) task the effects of stimulus similarity and the interval between a previous familiarization trial and such a task. 1h or 24 h after the familiarization trial with two identical copies of an object, exploration of one of the objects and another novel object was assessed by recording the number of approaches, as well the time spent close to them. On the test the stimuli could either differ slightly in form (Difficult discrimination condition) or markedly in form and color (Easy condition). Subjects in the Easy but not the Difficult condition preferred to explore the novel object regardless of the retention interval (1h or 24h). After the 24h interval, exploration of the novel stimulus was greater for subjects in the Easy than the Difficult condition but they did not differ in terms of exploring the familiar object. Thus, rats in the Difficult condition might remember the familiar stimuli as well as rats in the Easy condition after 24 h, but were unable to distinguish it from the novel stimulus. The implications of this notion for using the SOR task as a procedure for studying perceptual learning and memory are discussed
Bibliographic References
- Aggleton, J.P. Albasser, M.M., Aggleton, D.J., Poirier, G.L., & Pearce, M.J. (2010). Lesions on the rat perirhinal cortex spare the acquisition of a complex configural visual discrimination yet impair object recognition. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 975-983.
- Álvarez-Ruíz Y, & Carrillo-Mora P. (2013). Amyloid beta 25-35 impairs reconsolidation of object recognition memory in rats and this effect is prevented by lithium carbonate. Neuroscience Letters, 26, 548:79-83.
- Ameen-Ali K.E., Easton A., & Eacott MJ. (2015). Moving beyond standard procedures to assess spontaneous recognition memory. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 53, 37-51.
- Anderson, M.J. (2006a). Novel object recognition: assessing memory through exploratory responses. In: Anderson, M. J. (Ed.). Tasks and Techniques: A sampling of Methodologies for the Investigation of Animal Learning, Behavior, and Cognition. Nova Science Publishers Inc. Hauppauge, NY, pp.39-48.
- Anderson, M.J. (2006b). Object exploration: a non-aversive measure of object recognition, spatial memory, and context familiarity. In Hogan, S. N. (Ed.), Progress in Learning Research, Nova Science Publishers Inc. Hauppauge, NY, pp. 35-47.
- Anderson, M.J., Jablonsly, S. A., & Klimas, D. B. (2008). Spaced initial stimulus familiarization enhances novelty preference in Long-Evans rats. Behavioural Processes, 78, 481-486.
- Bennett, C. H., Wills, S. J., Wells, J. O., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1994). Reduced generalization following preexposure: Latent inhibition of common elements or a difference in familiarity? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 232-239.
- Berlyne, D. E. (1950). Novelty and curiosity as determinants of exploratory behavior. British Journal of Psychology. General Section, 41, 68-80.
- Bertaina-Anglade V, Enjuanes E, Morillon D, & Drieu la Rochelle C. (2006). The object recognition task in rats and mice: a simple and rapid model in safety pharmacology to detect amnesic properties of a new chemical entity. Pharmacol Toxicol Methods, 54, 99-105.
- Blaser R, & Heyser C. (2015). Spontaneous object recognition: a promising approach to the comparative study of memory. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9
- Burke SN, Wallace JL, Nematollahi S, Uprety AR, Barnes CA. (2010). Pattern separation deficits may contribute to age-associated recognition impairments. Behavioral Neuroscience, 124, 559-573.
- Burke SN, Wallace JL, Hartzell AL, Nematollahi S, Plange K, Barnes CA. (2011). Age-associated deficits in pattern separation functions of the perirhinal cortex: A cross-species consensus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 125, 836-847.
- Costa MS, Botton PH, Mioranzza S, Souza DO, & Porciuncula LO. (2008). Caffeine prevents age-associated recognition memory decline and changes brain-derived neurotrophic factor and tirosine kinase receptor (TrkB) content in mice. Neuroscience, 153, 1071-1078.
- Eacott, M. J., & Heywood, C.A. (1995). Perception and Memory: action and interaction. Crit. Rev. Neurobiol, 9, 311-320.
- Eacott MJ, Machin PE, Gaffan EA. (2001). Elemental and configural visual discrimination learning following lesions to perirhinal cortex in the rat. Behavioural Brain Research, 124, 55-70.
- Ennaceur, A. (2010). One-trial object recognition in rats and mice: Methodological and theoretical issues. Behavioural Brain Research, 215, 244-254.
- Ennaceur A, Delacour J. (1988). A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 31, 47-59.
- Gamiz, F. & Gallo, M. (2012). Spontaneous object recognition memory in aged rats: Complexity versus similarity. Learning and Memory, 19, 444-448.
- Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principals of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
- Hall, G. (2001). Perceptual learning: Association and differentiation. In: Mowrer, R.R., Klein, S.B. (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary learning theories. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 367-407.
- Honey, R. C., & Good, M. (2000). Associative modulation of the orienting response: Distinct effects revealed by hippocampal lesions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 3-14.
- Honey, R.C. & Hall, G. (1989). Enhanced discriminability and reduced associability following flavor preexposure. Learning and Motivation, 20, 262-277.
- Isono T, Yamashita N, Obara M, Araki T, Nakamura F, Kamiya Y, Alkam T, Nitta A, Nabeshima T, Mikoshiba K, Ohshima T, & Goshima Y. (2013). Amyloid-β25?35 induces impairment of cognitive function and long-term potentiation through phosphorylation of collapsin response mediator protein 2. Neuroscience Research, 77, 180-185.
- Mackintosh, N. J. (1987). Neurobiology, psychology and habituation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25, 81-97.
- Mackintosh, N. J., Kaye, H., & Bennett, C. H. (1991). Perceptual learning in flavour-aversion conditioning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43B, 297-322.
- Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological Review, 87, 252-271.
- Mitchell, C. & Hall, G. (2014). Can theories of animal discrimination explain perceptual learning in humans?. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 283-307.
- Norman, G. & Eacott, M.J. (2004). Impaired object recognition with increasing levels of feature ambiguity in rats with perirhinal cortex lesions. Behavioural Brain Research, 158, 79-91.
- Rodríguez, G. & Alonso, G. (2011). Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning. Learning & Behavior, 39, 79-86.
- Santos JR, Cunha JA, Dierschnabel AL, Campêlo CL, Leão AH, Silva AF, Engelberth RC, Izídio GS, Cavalcante JS, Abílio VC, Ribeiro AM, and Silva RH. (2013). Cognitive, motor and tyrosine hydroxylase temporal impairment in a model of parkinsonism induced by reserpine. Behavioural Brain Research, 15, 253:68-77
- Squire, L. R., Wixted, J. T., & Clark, R. E. (2007). Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: A new perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 872-883.
- Tam, S. K. E., Robinson, J., Jennings, D. J.; Bonardi, C. (2014). Dissociations in the effect of delay on object recognition: Evidence for an associative model of recognition memory. Journal of Experiment Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 40, 106-115.
- Whitt, E., & Robinson, J. 2013. Improved spontaneous object recognition following spaced preexposure trials: Evidence for an associative account of recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 39, 174-179.